logo
ResearchBunny Logo
Improving Outcome of Psychosocial Treatments by Enhancing Memory and Learning

Psychology

Improving Outcome of Psychosocial Treatments by Enhancing Memory and Learning

A. G. Harvey, J. Lee, et al.

Discover how enhancing memory can transform psychosocial treatments for mental disorders, as explored by Allison G. Harvey and her team from the University of California, Berkeley. This research delves into innovative pathways to boost treatment outcomes through cognitive strategies, sleep improvement, and more!

00:00
00:00
~3 min • Beginner • English
Introduction
The paper addresses whether improving patients’ memory for the content of psychosocial treatment sessions can substantially enhance clinical outcomes. Despite progress in evidence-based psychosocial interventions, many patients show limited or non-durable benefits. The rationale is grounded in: widespread memory deficits across psychiatric disorders, the modifiability of memory, the influence of emotion on encoding and retrieval, and evidence that patients recall little of professional advice or therapy content. The authors aim to synthesize cognitive psychology and education research to identify practical strategies that therapists can deploy to bolster encoding, consolidation, retrieval, and transfer of therapy ‘points’ into patients’ real-world behavior.
Literature Review
The review summarizes evidence of memory impairments across major depression, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, PTSD, and anxiety disorders (e.g., deficits in declarative and working memory; executive dysfunction; hippocampal/prefrontal disruptions; narrowed attention under negative affect). It highlights pervasive poor recall of medical recommendations: cancer patients accurately recall 23–33% of information; osteoporosis 31–63%; chronic pain about 30%; high cholesterol about 38%; recall of behavior change advice is particularly poor. Sparse studies in psychotherapy also show poor recall (e.g., insomnia patients forget ~1/3 of instructions; some items recalled as low as 13%; many genetic counseling patients forget recommendations or remember non-existent ones). Cognitive impairment predicts poorer psychosocial treatment outcomes and relapse risk; better baseline cognition predicts CBT completion and abstinence in substance use; memory difficulties relate to poorer CBT response in PTSD. Memory can be improved via cognitive support across conditions (e.g., Alzheimer’s/vascular dementia, older adults, depression). Theoretical foundations reviewed include: multicomponent working memory (phonological loop, visuospatial sketchpad, central executive, episodic buffer), levels-of-processing (deep semantic processing increases retention), interference (proactive and retroactive) and methods to mitigate it, stages of skill acquisition (cognitive→associative→autonomous), and transfer of learning challenges (importance of aligned contexts/cues).
Methodology
This is a conceptual and narrative review with translational synthesis. The authors iteratively: (a) reviewed cognitive psychology and education literatures on learning and memory, (b) coded psychotherapy session tapes to identify existing cognitive support techniques used by therapists, and (c) applied four criteria (identifiable, distinct, effective per literature, and actionable in-session) to select and define eight cognitive support strategies. They illustrate applications within sessions (dialogue examples), in handouts, between sessions (homework, phone/text prompts), and via digital delivery (internet/computerized interventions). They also outline additional mechanisms (sleep, exercise, pharmacological augmentation) that may enhance memory processes relevant to therapy.
Key Findings
- Five converging evidence streams suggest memory is a leverage point for improving psychotherapy outcomes: (1) memory deficits are common in mental illness; (2) memory is modifiable with cognitive support; (3) therapy activates emotion, which biases encoding (gist over detail); (4) emotion narrows attention, impairing registration of subsequent information; (5) therapy/medical advice recall is poor. - Empirical data on poor recall: cancer patients recall 23–33% of information; osteoporosis 31–63%; chronic pain ~30%; high cholesterol ~38%; insomnia therapy instructions—~1/3 forgotten, some categories as low as 13% recalled. - Sleep loss impairs encoding: a 40% reduction in new memory formation after sleep deprivation, disproportionately affecting neutral and positive emotional material; naps restore learning capacity. - Eight evidence-based, actionable strategies to enhance learning/memory in therapy: (1) attention recruitment; (2) categorization/chunking; (3) evaluation/self-explanation and comparison; (4) application via concrete episodic links and example-based/problem-directed learning; (5) repetition with spacing; (6) practice remembering (retrieval practice/testing effect, spaced retrieval, generative recall); (7) cue-based reminders (environmental cues, mnemonics, implementation intentions); (8) praising recall (reinforcement). - Practical implementations: structured handouts optimized for organizability (concentrated, concise, correspondent, concrete, coherent, comprehensible, codable), between-session prompts (texts/emails), session recordings, in vivo application, and digital delivery. - Additional pathways: optimize sleep before/after sessions to enhance encoding and consolidation/integration; exercise to support neuroplasticity (e.g., BDNF); pharmacologic augmentation (e.g., D-cycloserine) to enhance exposure-based learning in some anxiety disorders. - Potentially transdiagnostic and transtreatment utility, with particular promise for children and older adults given developmental differences in memory systems.
Discussion
The synthesis argues that enhancing memory for therapy points addresses core bottlenecks in psychosocial treatments: insufficient encoding under emotional arousal, limited depth of processing, interference from entrenched schemas and subsequent experiences, and failure to transfer learning to real-life contexts. The proposed strategies target these mechanisms: focusing attention at encoding, deepening processing via evaluation, organizing content to fit working memory constraints, rehearsing and retrieving to strengthen cues and semantic networks, and aligning cues across therapy and daily life to promote transfer. By integrating cognitive supports within and between sessions and leveraging technology, therapists can increase patients’ retention and application of skills, potentially improving adherence, generalization, and durability of gains. The approach is transdiagnostic, scalable, and compatible with existing modalities (e.g., CBT, DBT, behavioral activation), and may be tailored across the lifespan. Sleep and exercise offer adjunct avenues to optimize encoding and consolidation processes, while carefully selected pharmacologic enhancers may augment specific learning phases (e.g., extinction learning).
Conclusion
The paper contributes a theory-driven framework for improving psychosocial treatment outcomes by systematically enhancing patients’ memory and learning of therapy content. It defines eight actionable, evidence-backed cognitive support strategies and illustrates their integration within sessions, across homework/technology, and in digital therapies. It highlights additional modifiable factors (sleep, exercise, imagery) and potential pharmacologic augmentation. Future research should: (a) quantify current use of cognitive support across therapies and therapist experience levels; (b) develop and validate a taxonomy/measure of cognitive support; (c) test causal effects of specific strategies and optimal dosing/timing; (d) examine moderators (age, diagnosis, specific memory deficits) to tailor supports; (e) assess impacts on adherence, alliance, and long-term outcomes; and (f) evaluate feasibility, training burden, and cost-effectiveness in routine care and broader medical contexts.
Limitations
- Conceptual/narrative synthesis without randomized trials directly testing whether enhancing memory for therapy content improves clinical outcomes. - Potential variability in effectiveness across diagnoses (e.g., schizophrenia and bipolar disorder cognitive deficits may be less treatment-responsive than depression), requiring tailored strategies. - Unknown training burden, session time demands, and patient acceptability at scale; need to assess feasibility and costs. - Many potential confounds (age-related memory differences, baseline cognitive deficits, illness severity, homework compliance, therapeutic alliance) must be disentangled to isolate the effect of memory supports. - Generalization across diverse therapies and care settings remains to be empirically established; publication provides examples but not efficacy trials. - Publication date and specific implementation parameters (e.g., frequency/intensity of prompts) require further specification and testing.
Listen, Learn & Level Up
Over 10,000 hours of research content in 25+ fields, available in 12+ languages.
No more digging through PDFs, just hit play and absorb the world's latest research in your language, on your time.
listen to research audio papers with researchbunny